Skip to main content

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • Article
  • Published:

Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries

Abstract

People differ in their general tendency to endorse conspiracy theories (that is, conspiracy mentality). Previous research yielded inconsistent findings on the relationship between conspiracy mentality and political orientation, showing a greater conspiracy mentality either among the political right (a linear relation) or amongst both the left and right extremes (a curvilinear relation). We revisited this relationship across two studies spanning 26 countries (combined N = 104,253) and found overall evidence for both linear and quadratic relations, albeit small and heterogeneous across countries. We also observed stronger support for conspiracy mentality among voters of opposition parties (that is, those deprived of political control). Nonetheless, the quadratic effect of political orientation remained significant when adjusting for political control deprivation. We conclude that conspiracy mentality is associated with extreme left- and especially extreme right-wing beliefs, and that this non-linear relation may be strengthened by, but is not reducible to, deprivation of political control.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Buy this article

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Fig. 1: Linear relation of political orientation and conspiracy mentality (with 95% CI) in all samples separately and overall in multi-level models for both studies (controlling for quadratic relation).
Fig. 2: Quadratic relation of political orientation and conspiracy mentality (with 95% CI) in all samples separately and overall in multi-level models for both studies (controlling for linear relation).
Fig. 3: U-shaped relationship (tested with two-lines technique) of self-reported political orientation (raw) and conspiracy mentality in study 1 (N = 37,692).
Fig. 4: U-shaped relationship (tested with two-lines technique) of self-reported political orientation (raw) and conspiracy mentality in study 2 (N = 70,882).
Fig. 5: Conspiracy mentality as a function of linear and quadratic political orientation, inclusion of party intending to vote for in government (0 = no, 1 = yes) and their interaction in study 1 (N = 25,910) with predicted 95% confidence interval.
Fig. 6: Conspiracy mentality as a function of linear and quadratic political orientation, inclusion of party voted for in government (0 = no, 1 = yes) and their interaction in study 2 (N = 45,260) with predicted 95% confidence interval.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All data for study 1 and 2 are available at https://osf.io/jqnd6/.

Code availability

Custom code that supports the findings of this study is available as R markdown at https://osf.io/jqnd6/.

References

  1. Swami, V. et al. Conspiracist ideation in Britain and Austria: evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real‐world and fictitious conspiracy theories. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 443–463 (2011).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Douglas, K. M. COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 24, 270–275 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Imhoff, R. & Lamberty, P. A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 11, 1110–1118 (2020).

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Golec de Zavala, A. & Federico, C. M. Collective narcissism and the growth of conspiracy thinking over the course of the 2016 United States presidential election: a longitudinal analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 1011–1018 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Douglas, K. M. et al. Understanding conspiracy theories. Adv. Political Psychol. 40, 3–35 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. van Prooijen, J. W. The Psychology of Conspiracy Theories (Routledge, 2018).

  7. Butter, M. & Knight, P. (eds) Routledge Handbook of Conspiracy Theories (Routledge, 2020).

  8. Sunstein, C. R. & Vermeule, A. Conspiracy theories: causes and cures. J. Political Philos. 17, 202–227 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N. & Imhoff, R. Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Front. Psychol. 4, 225 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Goertzel, T. Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychol. 15, 731–742 (1994).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. M. Dead and alive: beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 3, 767–773 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Imhoff, R. & Bruder, M. Speaking (un-)truth to power: conspiracy mentality as a generalized political attitude. Eur. J. Personal. 28, 25–43 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Moscovici, S. in Changing Conceptions of Conspiracy (eds Graumann, C. F. & Moscovici, S.) 151–169 (Springer, 1987).

  14. Moscovici, S. Reflections on the popularity of ‘conspiracy mentalities’. Int. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 33, 1–12 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Imhoff, R. & Lamberty, P. Too special to be duped: need for uniqueness motivates conspiracy beliefs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 724–734 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Imhoff, R. & Lamberty, P. How paranoid are conspiracy believers? Towards a more fine-grained understanding of the connect and disconnect between paranoia and belief in conspiracy theories. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 909–926 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Bergmann, E. Conspiracy and Populism (Palgrave Macmillan, 2018).

  18. Müller, J. W. Was ist Populismus?: Ein Essay (Suhrkamp Verlag, 2016).

  19. Jolley, D. & Douglas, K. M. The social consequences of conspiracism: exposure to conspiracy theories decreases intentions to engage in politics and to reduce one’s carbon footprint. Br. J. Psychol. 105, 35–56 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Swami, V., Barron, D., Weis, L. & Furnham, A. To Brexit or not to Brexit: the roles of Islamophobia, conspiracist beliefs, and integrated threat in voting intentions for the United Kingdom European Union membership referendum. Br. J. Psychol. 109, 156–179 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Imhoff, R., Dieterle, L. & Lamberty, P. Resolving the puzzle of conspiracy worldview and political activism: belief in secret plots decreases normative but increases non-normative political engagement. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 12, 71–79 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Jolley, D. & Paterson, J. L. Pylons ablaze: examining the role of 5G COVID‐19 conspiracy beliefs and support for violence. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 59, 628–640 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J. & Sanford, R. N. The Authoritarian Personality (Harper & Brothers, 1950).

  24. Imhoff, R. in The Psychology of Conspiracy (eds Bilewicz, M., Cichocka, A. & Soral, W.) 122–141 (Routledge, 2015).

  25. Abalakina‐Paap, M., Stephan, W. G., Craig, T. & Gregory, W. L. Beliefs in conspiracies. Political Psychol. 20, 637–647 (1999).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Đorđević, J. M., Žeželj, I. & Đurić, Ž. Beyond general political attitudes: conspiracy mentality as a global belief system predicts endorsement of international and local conspiracy theories. J. Soc. Political Psychol. 9, 144–158 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Grzesiak-Feldman, M. & Irzycka, M. Right-wing authoritarianism and conspiracy thinking in a Polish sample. Psychol. Rep. 105, 389–393 (2009).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Dieguez, S., Wagner-Egger, P. & Gauvrit, N. Nothing happens by accident, or does it? A low prior for randomness does not explain belief in conspiracy theories. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1762–1770 (2015).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J. & Harvey, A. J. Someone is pulling the strings: hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Think. Reason. 22, 57–77 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Gauchat, G. Politicization of science in the public sphere: a study of public trust in the United States, 1974 to 2010. Am. Sociol. Rev. 77, 167–187 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Jost, J. T., van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C. & Hardin, C. Ideological asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of misinformation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 23, 77–83 (2018).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L. & Farhart, C. E. Conspiracy endorsement as motivated reasoning: the moderating roles of political knowledge and trust. Am. J. Political Sci. 60, 824–844 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C., Azevedo, F. & Jost, J. T. The paranoid style in American politics revisited: an ideological asymmetry in conspiratorial thinking. Political Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12681 (2020).

  34. van Prooijen, J. W., Krouwel, A. P. & Pollet, T. V. Political extremism predicts belief in conspiracy theories. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 6, 570–578 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Nera, K., Wagner-Egger, P., Bertin, P., Douglas, K. & Klein, O. A power-challenging theory of society, or a conservative mindset? Upward and downward conspiracy theories as ideologically distinct beliefs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2769 (2021).

  36. Krouwel, A., Kutiyski, Y., Van Prooijen, J. W., Martinsson, J. & Markstedt, E. Does extreme political ideology predict conspiracy beliefs, economic evaluations and political trust? Evidence from Sweden. J. Soc. Political Psychol. 5, 435–462 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Babińska, M. & Bilewicz, M. in Uprzedzenia w Polsce 2017 [Prejudice in Poland 2017] (eds Stefaniak, A & Winiewski, M.) 307–327 (Liberi Libri, 2019).

  38. Imhoff, R. & Decker, O. in Rechtsextremismus der Mitte (eds Decker O., Kiess J. & Brähler, E.) 130–145 (Psychosozial Verlag, 2013).

  39. Arendt, H. The Origins of Totalitarianism Part 1: Antisemitism (Harcourt, Brace and World, 1951).

  40. Brandt, M. J., Reyna, C., Chambers, J. R., Crawford, J. T. & Wetherell, G. The ideological-conflict hypothesis: intolerance among both liberals and conservatives. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 27–34 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Cichocka, A., Bilewicz, M., Jost, J. T., Marrouch, N. & Witkowska, M. On the grammar of politics—or why conservatives prefer nouns. Political Psychol. 37, 799–815 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Koch, A., Dorrough, A., Glöckner, A. & Imhoff, R. The ABC of society: perceived similarity in agency/socioeconomic success and conservative-progressive beliefs increases intergroup cooperation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 90, 103996 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Koch, A. et al. Groups’ warmth is a personal matter: understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 89, 103995 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Sternisko, A., Cichocka, A. & van Bavel, J. The dark side of social movements: social identity, non-conformity, and the lure of conspiracy theories. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 35, 1–6 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. van Prooijen, J. W. & Krouwel, A. P. Extreme political beliefs predict dogmatic intolerance. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 8, 292–300 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. van Prooijen, J. W. & Krouwel, A. P. Psychological features of extreme political ideologies. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28, 159–163 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Rooduijn, M. & Akkerman, T. Flank attacks: populism and left–right radicalism in Western Europe. Part. Politics 23, 193–204 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Oliver, J. E. & Wood, T. J. Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. Am. J. Political Sci. 58, 952–966 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Conway, L. G. III, Houck, S. C., Gornick, L. J. & Repke, M. A. Finding the Loch Ness monster: left‐wing authoritarianism in the United States. Political Psychol. 39, 1049–1067 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Van Hiel, A., Duriez, B. & Kossowska, M. The presence of left‐wing authoritarianism in Western Europe and its relationship with conservative ideology. Political Psychol. 27, 769–793 (2006).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Bartlett, J. & Miller, C. The Power of Unreason: Conspiracy Theories, Extremism and Counter-terrorism (Demos, 2010).

  52. Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M. & Cichocka, A. The psychology of conspiracy theories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 538–542 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Sullivan, D., Landau, M. J. & Rothschild, Z. K. An existential function of enemyship: evidence that people attribute influence to personal and political enemies to compensate for threats to control. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 98, 434–449 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. van Prooijen, J. W. & Acker, M. The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: conceptual and applied extensions. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 753–761 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Stojanov, A. & Halberstadt, J. Does lack of control lead to conspiracy beliefs? A meta‐analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 50, 955–968 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Kofta, M., Soral, W. & Bilewicz, M. What breeds conspiracy antisemitism? The role of political uncontrollability and uncertainty in the belief in Jewish conspiracy. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 118, 900–918 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Edelson, J., Alduncin, A., Krewson, C., Sieja, J. A. & Uscinski, J. E. The effect of conspiratorial thinking and motivated reasoning on belief in election fraud. Political Res. Q. 70, 933–946 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Uscinski, J. E. & Parent, J. M. American Conspiracy Theories (Oxford Univ. Press, 2014).

  59. Nyhan, B. Media scandals are political events: how contextual factors affect public controversies over alleged misconduct by US governors. Political Res. Q. 70, 223–236 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A. & Gosling, S. D. Ideology: its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 126–136 (2008).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Furnham, A. & Fenton-O’Creevy, M. Personality and political orientation. Personal. Individ. Diff. 129, 88–91 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Sibley, C. G., Osborne, D. & Duckitt, J. Personality and political orientation: meta-analysis and test of a threat-constraint model. J. Res. Personal. 46, 664–677 (2012).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W. & Sulloway, F. J. Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychol. Bull. 129, 339–375 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Huber, J. D. Values and partisanship in left–right orientations: measuring ideology. Eur. J. Political Res. 17, 599–621 (1989).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Bauer, P. C., Barberá, P., Ackermann, K. & Venetz, A. Is the left–right scale a valid measure of ideology? Political Behav. 39, 553–583 (2017).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Malka, A., Soto, C. J., Inzlicht, M. & Lelkes, Y. Do needs for security and certainty predict cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national analysis. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 106, 1031–1051 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Bakker, R. et al. 2014 Chapel Hill Expert Survey. University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill www.chesdata.eu (2015).

  68. Simonsohn, U. Two lines: a valid alternative to the invalid testing of U-shaped relationships with quadratic regressions. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci. 1, 538–555 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Jolley, D., Douglas, K. M. & Sutton, R. M. Blaming a few bad apples to save a threatened barrel: the system‐justifying function of conspiracy theories. Political Psychol. 39, 465–478 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Leone, L., Giacomantonio, M. & Lauriola, M. Moral foundations, worldviews, moral absolutism and belief in conspiracy theories. Int. J. Psychol. 54, 197–204 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Marchlewska, M., Cichocka, A. & Kossowska, M. Addicted to answers: need for cognitive closure and the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 109–117 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Cichocka, A., Marchlewska, M., Golec de Zavala, A. & Olechowski, M. “They will not control us”: in-group positivity and belief in intergroup conspiracies. Br. J. Psychol. 107, 556–576 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Imhoff, R. & Lamberty, P. in Routledge Handbook of Conspiracy Theories (eds Butter, M. & Knight, P.) 192–205 (Routledge, 2020).

  74. Morisi, D., Jost, J. T. & Singh, V. An asymmetrical “President-in-power” effect. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 113, 614–620 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. van Prooijen, J. ‐W. Why education predicts decreased belief in conspiracy theories. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 31, 50–58 (2017).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Bognar, E. in Reuters Institute Digital News Report 2018 (eds Newman, N. et al.) 84–85 (Reuters, 2018).

  77. Összeesküvés-elméletek, álhírek, Babonák a Magyar közvelemenyben. Political Capital https://politicalcapital.hu/pc-admin/source/documents/pc-boll-konteo-20181107.pdf (2018).

  78. Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct (2002, amended effective June 1, 2010, and January 1, 2017). American Psychological Association http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx (2017).

  79. Krouwel, A., Kutiyski, Y. & Thomeczek, P. EVES: European Voter Election Studies Survey Data (Kieskompas, 2019).

  80. Rao, J. N. K., Yung, W. & Hidiroglou, M. A. Estimating equations for the analysis of survey data using poststratification information. Sankhyā: Indian J. Stat. A 364–378 (2002).

  81. Valliant, R. Poststratification and conditional variance estimation. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 88, 89–96 (1993).

    Google Scholar 

  82. Rutkowski, L. & Svetina, D. Assessing the hypothesis of measurement invariance in the context of large-scale international surveys. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 74, 31–57 (2014).

  83. Richter, D. & Schupp, J. The SOEP Innovation Sample (SOEP IS). Schmollers Jahrb. 135, 389–399 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work has been coordinated, presented and discussed within the framework of EU COST Action CA15101 ‘Comparative Analysis of Conspiracy Theories (COMPACT)’. German data stem from the 2014 Innovation Sample of the Socio-Economic Panel83 (SOEP). Data from the Andalusian survey conducted in Spain come from the research project ‘Conspiracy Theories and Disinformation’ directed by Estrella Gualda (University of Huelva, Spain), whose fieldwork was supported and executed by the Institute of Advanced Social Studies (IESA-CSIC) in the context of a grant received for executing the 5th Wave of the Citizen’s Panel Survey for Social Research in Andalusia (ref. EP-1707, PIE 201710E018, IESA/CSIC, https://panelpacis.net/; E.G.). The Czech part of the study was supported by grant 20-01214S (S.G.) from the Czech Science Foundation and by RVO: 68081740 (S.G.) of the Institute of Psychology, Czech Academy of Sciences. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

R.I. and J.-W.v.P. initiated the project with an open call. R.I., O.K., J.H.C.A., M.Bi., A.B., M.Ba., N.B., K.B., R.B., A.C., S.D., K.M.D., A.D., B.G., S.G., G.H., A.K., P.K., A.K., S.M., J.M.D., M.S.P., M.P., L.P., G.P., A.R., R.N.R., F.A.S., M.S., R.M.S., V.S., H.T., V.T., P.W.-E., I.Ž. and J.-W.v.P. contributed to the conception of study 1 and collected data in their respective country. A.K., Y.K. and T.E. provided the data for study 2. R.I. and F.Z. analysed and interpreted the data with helpful input from O.K. R.I. drafted the article. All authors provided critical revision and approved the final version of the article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Roland Imhoff.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks Bruno Castanho Silva, Federico Vegetti and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Supplementary information

Supplementary Sects. 1–12 and Tables S1–S25.

Reporting summary.

Peer review information.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Imhoff, R., Zimmer, F., Klein, O. et al. Conspiracy mentality and political orientation across 26 countries. Nat Hum Behav 6, 392–403 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01258-7

Search

Quick links

Nature Briefing

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

Get the most important science stories of the day, free in your inbox. Sign up for Nature Briefing